Page Not Found - IPRA Foundation

404


Sorry - Page Not Found!

The page you are looking for was moved, removed, renamed
or might never existed. You stumbled upon a broken link :

© 2024 IPRA Foundation. All Rights Reserved.

Page Not Found - IPRA Foundation

404


Sorry - Page Not Found!

The page you are looking for was moved, removed, renamed
or might never existed. You stumbled upon a broken link :

© 2024 IPRA Foundation. All Rights Reserved.

SMALL PEACE RESEARCH GRANTS

GRANT AWARDEE: Rezarta Bilali (Albania)

The Effect of Identity on Memories of Group Conflict

Perceptions of history and collective memories are important in maintaining and perpetuating intergroup conflicts. Such views of history have a very strong impact on group members’ current perceptions of conflict and prospects for its resolution. Despite the shared nature of these beliefs, group members often exhibit differences in their construals of past collective events of intergroup conflict. Understanding the conditions and processes that lead to differential construals of the past across and within groups is important as it might provide insights for successful interventions.

Three questions drove this research. First, how do group members construe violent intergroup conflicts involving their ingroup? Second, does the degree of ingroup identification influence individuals’ interpretations of these conflicts? Third, what is the role of different types of ingroup identification (ethnic vs. national) in these construals? These issues were investigated in three different contexts of intergroup conflicts, in Turkey and Burundi: (1) the Turkish-Armenian conflict and mass killings between 1880s-1920s, (2) the Turkish-Kurdish conflict in Turkey between 1984 to 2005, and (3) the ongoing ethnic conflict between Hutus and Tutsis in Burundi since 1962.

Drawing from previous work, I identified and assessed three dimensions that contribute to differential construals of intergroup conflict: attributions of responsibility (ingroup vs. outgroup vs. external factors), perceived severity of harm (on the ingroup vs. the outgroup), and conflict framing. I investigated construals of conflict by examining cross-group and within-group variations in these three dimensions.

The first study examined how group members construe a historical period of intergroup conflict and violence, and whether variations in the strength of ingroup identification predict these construals. These issues were examined in the context of Turkish-Armenian conflict by investigating Turks’ construal of events related to the mass-killings of Armenians in the period between 1880 – 1920. Participants in this research were 113 Turkish students recruited at various universities in the United States. All participants completed a survey instrument administered in Turkish. The first part of the instrument assessed participants’ ingroup (Turkish) identification. A second section assessed participants’ construal of the past conflict between Armenians and Turks between 1880s – 1920s.

Study 2 furthered this investigation in a different conflict context (Turkish-Kurdish conflict) by examining whether the influence of ingroup identification on interpretations of past violent intergroup events varies as a function of one’s group membership and status (Turks vs. Kurds; majority vs. minority). Study 2 extended Study 1 by: (1) comparing both groups’ (Turks and Kurds) perspectives of the conflict, (3) assessing an additional dimension of construals, conflict framing (i.e., framing as terrorism, minority issue, or ethnic conflict) (4) moving beyond ingroup and outgroup distinctions to assess which types of targets (insurgent groups, government, and general population) affiliated with the in- or outgroup were held responsible for the conflict, and (5) examining the effect of different types of group identification (ethnic vs. national identification) on construals of the conflict. Participants were 298 Turkish nationals (194 ethnic Turks and 80 ethnic Kurds) living in Turkey. The procedures were similar to those in Study 1.

Study 3 replicated Study 2 in a more intense ongoing conflict context: the ethnic conflict in Burundi. Participants were 114 (56 Hutus, 58 Tutsis) students at two different universities in Bujumbura. The methods were similar to the first two studies.

Results

Across the three studies participants assigned less responsibility to their ingroup, but more responsibility to the outgroup and to the external factors (such as third parties or external circumstances), regardless of whether the ingroup was a perpetrator (e.g., Turks in the Armenian genocide) or a victim (e.g., Kurds in the Turkish-Kurdish conflict, or Hutus in Burundi until 1990s). Across studies, respondents of each group assigned some responsibility to the ingroup. Interestingly, participants also assigned substantial responsibility to third parties and other external factors for the conflict. For instance, Turks attributed equal amount of responsibility to external factors as to Armenians for the massacres of 1915. Similarly, both Turks and Kurds assigned more responsibility to foreign countries than to the respective ingroup (Turkish and Kurdish people) for the Turkish-Kurdish conflict. The current results suggest that third parties and situational factors play an important role in group members’ beliefs about initiation and evolution of conflict.

In my research I distinguished between two types of responsibility: responsibility for the instigation versus responsibility for the consequences of the events. Each group in a conflict is likely to perceive their acts of violence as a response to threat or provocation by the outgroup. Perceiving the ingroup as less responsible for instigating the conflict and perceiving the outgroup as the instigator of the conflict might provide moral justification for ingroup’s violent acts. Studies 1 and 3 indicated that groups engage in a competition over ‘who started the conflict’. Participants attributed less responsibility to the ingroup for the instigation than for the consequences of the conflict, and led the outgroup to be perceived as more responsible for the instigation than for the consequences of the conflict. To deflect their responsibility, perpetrator groups might be more motivated to focus on the responsibility for the instigation of the acts; whereas victims, being preoccupied with the harm and suffering inflicted upon them, might focus primarily on the responsibility for the consequences of the conflict.

Comparisons of the amount of harm inflicted on the ingroup and the outgroup yielded different results across the three studies. Turkish respondents reported that Armenians have suffered more than Turks during the Turkish-Armenian conflict; Turkish respondents perceived equal amount of harm to be inflicted on Turks and Kurds during the Turkish-Kurdish conflict, whereas Kurds perceived their ingroup to have suffered more; similarly, Hutus and Tutsis perceived the respective ingroups to have suffered more during the conflict. These results were interpreted by taking into account the social reality constraints of each conflict context under study.

Across studies, supporting my hypotheses, stronger identification with the ethnic group was related to (1) attributing more responsibility to outgroup targets and external factors and less responsibility to ingroup targets, (2) more harm perceived to be inflicted on the ingroup and less harm perceived to be inflicted on the outgroup as well as to (3) higher endorsement of conflict frames that portray the ingroup in a positive light.

For Turks, national identification was strongly correlated with ethnic identification, and consequently it paralleled the effects of ethnic identification on construals of conflict. However, the degree to which Kurdish respondents identified with their ethnic group was not associated with the degree to which they identified with the Turkish nation. For Kurdish respondents, identification with the Turkish nation produced outgroup favoritism (e.g., perceiving more ingroup responsibility and less outgroup responsibility for the conflict).

Conclusion

Burundi, Turkey and other societies that have experienced violent conflicts face important decisions regarding how to deal with their violent history. How can truth about the past be established when groups endorse such different interpretations of the past? Reconciliation efforts around the world have adopted programs such as truth and reconciliation commissions (TRCs) that aim to address and deal with the past as a way to reconstruct the future. However, in order to deal with these issues in an effective way it is important to understand how groups and their members construe their conflicts and how they come to different understandings of the past. Understanding the factors that lead to different construals and interpretations is important to inform effective strategies to change them. The current research contributed to this understanding.

IPRA Grant

I thank International Peace Research Association for supporting this research. Originally, I had planned to conduct this research only in Turkey, by examining Turkish-Kurdish and Turkish-Armenian conflict contexts. This grant was instrumental not only in completing the planned research, but it also made possible to extend the research to the investigation of the ethnic conflict in Burundi. The IPRA grant was used to collect research data in Turkey and in Burundi.

Contact Information

Rezarta Bilali, Ph.D.
Assistant Professor
Graduate Programs in Dispute Resolution
University of Massachusetts Boston
100 Morrissey Blvd.
Wheatley Hall
Boston, MA02125
Tel: +16172877165
Fax: +16172877412

 

Rezarta Bilali completed her Ph.D. at the University of Massachusetts in Amherst in Social Psychology with a concentration on Peace and Violence at the University of Massachusetts Amherst. Her dissertation is entitled “The Effect of Group Identity on Memories of Past Conflicts.” While she was a graduate student she received a Guggenheim award for intergroup conflict research.

She is currently an assistant professor at the University of Massachusetts at Boston in Dispute Resolution. Here are some of her publications:

Bilali, R. (in press). Assessing the internal validity of image theory in the context of Turkey-U.S. relations. Political Psychology.

Bilali, R. (in press). Image Theory. In Christie, D. (Ed.). Encyclopedia of Peace Psychology. Blackwell Publishing.

Bilali, R. (in press). Collective Memories. In Christie, D. (Ed.). Encyclopedia of Peace Psychology. Blackwell Publishing.

Staub, E., Pearlman, L., & Bilali, R. (2009). Understanding the roots and impact of violence and psychological recovery as avenues to reconciliation after mass violence and intractable conflict: applications in Rwanda. In G. Solomon & E. Cairns (Eds.). Handbook of Peace Education. Psychology Press: Taylor & Francis.

Vollhardt, J. & Bilali, R. (2008). Social psychology’s contribution to the psychological study of peace: a review. Social Psychology, 39, 12-25.

Staub, E., Pearlman, L., & Bilali, R. (2008). Psychological recovery, reconciliation and the prevention of new violence: an approach and its uses in Rwanda. In B. Hart (Ed.). Peacebuilding in Traumatized Societies. American University Press.